El test de creatividad (TCI)evaluando la creatividad mediante una tarea de encontrar problemas

  1. Manuela Romo 1
  2. Vicente Alfonso-Benlliure 2
  3. Maria-Jose Sanchez-Ruiz 3
  1. 1 Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
    info

    Universidad Autónoma de Madrid

    Madrid, España

    ROR https://ror.org/01cby8j38

  2. 2 Universitat de València
    info

    Universitat de València

    Valencia, España

    ROR https://ror.org/043nxc105

  3. 3 Lebanese American University
    info

    Lebanese American University

    Beirut, Líbano

    ROR https://ror.org/00hqkan37

Revista:
Psicología educativa

ISSN: 1135-755X

Año de publicación: 2016

Volumen: 22

Número: 2

Páginas: 93-101

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.1016/J.PSE.2016.01.005 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: Psicología educativa

Resumen

Los tests de pensamiento divergente no atienden generalmente a la naturaleza compleja de la creatividad y se centran en el producto final o solución del problema, obviando fases previas del proceso creativo como la búsqueda y formulación del problema. El presente estudio adopta el modelo de problem-finding y plantea una nueva medida de la creatividad para ninos ˜ de educación primaria (6-12 anos). ˜ Se expone la fundamentación teórica así como el proceso de diseno, ˜ construcción y validación de la prueba a través de diferentes estudios. El Test de Creatividad Infantil (TCI) evalúa el proceso creativo a partir de una tarea estructurada en dos fases: formulación y solución del problema. El test considera no sólo el resultado final (un dibujo) sino las fases previas que llevan a alcanzarlo. Los resultados muestran una fiabilidad y validez satisfactorias de las puntuaciones de la prueba. Se concluye que el TCI tiene unas propiedades psicométricas robustas y puede ser una herramienta útil para predecir el comportamiento creativo de los ninos ˜ en educación primaria.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context. New York: Westview.
  • Artola, T., Ancillo., Barraca, J. y Mosteiro, P. (2004, 2010). PIC-N. Prueba de Imaginación Creativa para Niños. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Batey, M. y Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, General and Social Psychology Monographs, 132, 355–429.
  • Besançon, M. y Lubart, T. I. (2008). Differences in the development of creative competencies in children schooled in diverse learning environments. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 381–389.
  • Charles, R. E. y Runco, M. A. (2001). Developmental trends in the evaluative and divergent thinking of children. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 417–437.
  • Chase, C. I. (1985). Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. En J. V. Mitchell Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (pp. 1631–1632). Lincoln: University of Nebraska. Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
  • Corbalán, J., Martínez, F., Donolo, D. S., Alonso, C., Tejerina, M. y Limiñana, R. M.(2003). CREA. Inteligencia creativa. Una medida cognitiva de la creatividad. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Cropley, A. (2001). Creativity in education and learning. London: Kogan Page. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1998). Creatividad. El fluir y la Psicología del descubrimiento y la invención. Barcelona: Paidós.
  • Dillon, J. T. (1982). Problem finding and solving. Journal of Creative Behavior, 16(12), 97–112.
  • Fairweather, E. y Cramond, B. (2010). Infusing creative and critical thinking into the curriculum together. En R. R. Beghetto y J. Kaufmann (Eds.), Nurturing creativity in the classroom (pp. 113–141). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Feldman. (1999). The development of creativity. En R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 169–186). Cambridge Unviersity Press.
  • Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2ndEd). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Gardner, H. (1982). Art, mind, and brain: A cognitive approach to creativity. New York. NY: Basic Books.
  • Getzels, J. W. y Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Getzels, J. W. y Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence: Exploration with gifted students. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Gopnik, A. (2009). The Philosophical Baby: What children’s minds tell us about truth, love and the meaning of life. New York, NY: Farrar, Strauss, and Giroux; London: The Bodley Head.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1956). Structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin., 53, 267–293.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: Mac Graw-Hill.
  • Guilford, J. P. y Hoepfner, R. (1971). The analysis of intelligence. Nueva York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Hayes, J. R. (1981). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press.
  • Hocevar, D. y Bachelord, P. (1989). A taxonomy and critique of measurements used in the study of creativity. En J. A. Glober, R. R. Ronning, y C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of Creativity. (pp. 53–75). New York, NY: Plenun press.
  • Jay, E. S. y Perkins, D. N. (1997). Problem finding: The search for mechanisms. En M. A. Runco (Ed.), The creativity research handbook (pp. 257–293). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
  • Kim, K. H. (2006). Can we trust creativity tests? A review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT). Creativity Research Journal, 18, 3–14.
  • Kim, K. H. (2011). The creativity crisis: The decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 285–295.
  • Lubart, T. I. y Lautrey, J. (1995). Relationships between creative development and cognitive development. In Paper presented at the 7th European Conference on Developmental Psychology.
  • Lubart, T., Mouchiroud, C., Tordjman, S. y Zenasni, F. (2003). Psychologie de la créativité. Paris: Armand Colin.
  • Maker, C. J., Jo, S. y Muammar, O. M. (2008). Development of creativity: The influence of varying levels of implementation of the DISCOVER curriculum model, a non-traditional pedagogical approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 18,402–417.
  • Moore, M. T. (1985). Problem finding and the teacher experience. Journal of creative behavior, 24, 39–58.
  • Mullineaux, P. y Dilalla, L. F. (2009). Preschool pretend play behaviors and early adolescent creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 43, 41–57.
  • Newell, A. y Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. New York: Prentice Hall.
  • Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A. y Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 145–153.
  • Perkins, D. (1981). The mind best work. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press. Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance’s (1958 to present) longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 103–114.
  • Plucker, J. y Renzulli, J. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the study of human creativity. En R. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 35–61). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Romo, M. y Alfonso-Benlliure, V. (2010). Viabilidad del modelo de Encontrar Problemas para evaluar la creatividad en Educación Primaria. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 33, 335–349.
  • Romo, M., Alfonso-Benlliure, V. y Sánchez Ruiz, M. J. (2008). Test de Creatividad Infantil. Madrid: Tea Ediciones.
  • Rosenblatt, E. y Winner, E. (1988). The art of children’s drawings. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 22, 3–15.
  • Runco, M. (1994). Problem finding, problem solving and creativity. Norwood. NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
  • Runco, M. (2003). Idea evaluation, divergent thinking, and creativity. En M. A. Runco (Ed.), Critical creative processes (pp. 69–74). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
  • Runco, M. A. (2008). Commentary: Divergent thinking is not synonymous with creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 93–96.
  • Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Emergence in creativity and development. En R. K. Sawyer,V. John-Steiner, S. Moran, R. Sternberg, D. H. Feldman, M. Csikszentmihalyi, y J. Nakamura (Eds.), Creativity and Development (pp. 12–60). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Sawyer, R. K. (2012). Explaining creativity: The science of human innovation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Smith, G. J. y Carlsson, I. M. (1983). Creativity in early and middle school years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 167–195.
  • Smith, G. J. y Carlsson, I. M. (1985). Creativity in middle and late school years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 329–343.
  • Smith, G. J. y Carlsson, I. M. (1990). The creative process: A functional model based on empirical studies from early childhood to middle age. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
  • Smolucha, F. (1992). A reconstruction of Vygotsky’s theory of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 5.
  • Sternberg, R. (1988). The nature of creativity. Contemporary psychological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sternberg, R. (1999). Handbook of creativity. New York. NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sternberg, R. y Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51, 677–688.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth-grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12, 195–199.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–252.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1974). Norms-technical manual: Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Lexington, MA: Personel Press/Ginn.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing. En R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity, Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 43–75). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1990). The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms - technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1998). The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms - technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
  • Treffinger, D. J. (1985). Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. En J. V. Mitchell Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (pp. 1632–1634). Lincoln: University of Nebraska. Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
  • Urban, K. K. (1991). On the development of creativity in children. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 177–191.
  • Urban, K. K. (2004). Assessing Creativity: The Test for Creative Thinking-Drawing Production (TCT-DP). The Concept, Application, Evaluation and International Studies. Psychology Science, 46, 387–397.
  • Urban, K. K. y Jellen, H. G. (1996). Test for Creative Thinking - Drawing Production (TCTDP). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1930-2004). Imagination and creativity in childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42(1), 4–84.
  • Wallach, M. A. y Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children. New York, NY: Holt.
  • Wakefield, J. F. (1991). The outlook for creativity tests. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 25, 184–193.
  • Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. Hoboken, New York, NY: John Wiley.
  • Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper and Brothers. (Trad. Cast. de L. Wolfson, El pensamiento productivo. Barcelona: Paidós, 1991).
  • Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W. y Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 24–37.