El test de creatividad (TCI)evaluando la creatividad mediante una tarea de encontrar problemas

  1. Manuela Romo 1
  2. Vicente Alfonso-Benlliure 2
  3. Maria-Jose Sanchez-Ruiz 3
  1. 1 Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
    info

    Universidad Autónoma de Madrid

    Madrid, España

    ROR https://ror.org/01cby8j38

  2. 2 Universitat de València
    info

    Universitat de València

    Valencia, España

    ROR https://ror.org/043nxc105

  3. 3 Lebanese American University
    info

    Lebanese American University

    Beirut, Líbano

    ROR https://ror.org/00hqkan37

Zeitschrift:
Psicología educativa

ISSN: 1135-755X

Datum der Publikation: 2016

Ausgabe: 22

Nummer: 2

Seiten: 93-101

Art: Artikel

DOI: 10.1016/J.PSE.2016.01.005 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openOpen Access editor

Andere Publikationen in: Psicología educativa

Zusammenfassung

Divergent thinking tests do not generally address the complex nature of creativity but rather focus on the final product or solution of a problem, overlooking the previous stages of the creative process such as the discovery and formulation of a problem. The present study adopts the lrsquo;problem finding’ model and presents a new measure of creativity in children in primary education (6-12 years old). This paper presents the theoretical foundations as well as the process of designing, developing, and validating the test through different studies. The Child Creativity Test (TCI in Spanish) evaluates the creative process through a task structured in two stages: formulation and solution of a problem. The test considers not only the final output (a drawing), but also the previous phases that lead to it. Results show satisfactory validity and reliability of the test scores. It is concluded that the TCI has robust psychometric properties and can be a useful tool to predict creative behavior in primary school children.

Bibliographische Referenzen

  • Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context. New York: Westview.
  • Artola, T., Ancillo., Barraca, J. y Mosteiro, P. (2004, 2010). PIC-N. Prueba de Imaginación Creativa para Niños. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Batey, M. y Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, General and Social Psychology Monographs, 132, 355–429.
  • Besançon, M. y Lubart, T. I. (2008). Differences in the development of creative competencies in children schooled in diverse learning environments. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 381–389.
  • Charles, R. E. y Runco, M. A. (2001). Developmental trends in the evaluative and divergent thinking of children. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 417–437.
  • Chase, C. I. (1985). Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. En J. V. Mitchell Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (pp. 1631–1632). Lincoln: University of Nebraska. Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
  • Corbalán, J., Martínez, F., Donolo, D. S., Alonso, C., Tejerina, M. y Limiñana, R. M.(2003). CREA. Inteligencia creativa. Una medida cognitiva de la creatividad. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Cropley, A. (2001). Creativity in education and learning. London: Kogan Page. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1998). Creatividad. El fluir y la Psicología del descubrimiento y la invención. Barcelona: Paidós.
  • Dillon, J. T. (1982). Problem finding and solving. Journal of Creative Behavior, 16(12), 97–112.
  • Fairweather, E. y Cramond, B. (2010). Infusing creative and critical thinking into the curriculum together. En R. R. Beghetto y J. Kaufmann (Eds.), Nurturing creativity in the classroom (pp. 113–141). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Feldman. (1999). The development of creativity. En R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 169–186). Cambridge Unviersity Press.
  • Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2ndEd). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Gardner, H. (1982). Art, mind, and brain: A cognitive approach to creativity. New York. NY: Basic Books.
  • Getzels, J. W. y Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Getzels, J. W. y Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence: Exploration with gifted students. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Gopnik, A. (2009). The Philosophical Baby: What children’s minds tell us about truth, love and the meaning of life. New York, NY: Farrar, Strauss, and Giroux; London: The Bodley Head.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1956). Structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin., 53, 267–293.
  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: Mac Graw-Hill.
  • Guilford, J. P. y Hoepfner, R. (1971). The analysis of intelligence. Nueva York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Hayes, J. R. (1981). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press.
  • Hocevar, D. y Bachelord, P. (1989). A taxonomy and critique of measurements used in the study of creativity. En J. A. Glober, R. R. Ronning, y C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of Creativity. (pp. 53–75). New York, NY: Plenun press.
  • Jay, E. S. y Perkins, D. N. (1997). Problem finding: The search for mechanisms. En M. A. Runco (Ed.), The creativity research handbook (pp. 257–293). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
  • Kim, K. H. (2006). Can we trust creativity tests? A review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT). Creativity Research Journal, 18, 3–14.
  • Kim, K. H. (2011). The creativity crisis: The decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 285–295.
  • Lubart, T. I. y Lautrey, J. (1995). Relationships between creative development and cognitive development. In Paper presented at the 7th European Conference on Developmental Psychology.
  • Lubart, T., Mouchiroud, C., Tordjman, S. y Zenasni, F. (2003). Psychologie de la créativité. Paris: Armand Colin.
  • Maker, C. J., Jo, S. y Muammar, O. M. (2008). Development of creativity: The influence of varying levels of implementation of the DISCOVER curriculum model, a non-traditional pedagogical approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 18,402–417.
  • Moore, M. T. (1985). Problem finding and the teacher experience. Journal of creative behavior, 24, 39–58.
  • Mullineaux, P. y Dilalla, L. F. (2009). Preschool pretend play behaviors and early adolescent creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 43, 41–57.
  • Newell, A. y Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. New York: Prentice Hall.
  • Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A. y Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 145–153.
  • Perkins, D. (1981). The mind best work. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press. Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance’s (1958 to present) longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 103–114.
  • Plucker, J. y Renzulli, J. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the study of human creativity. En R. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 35–61). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Romo, M. y Alfonso-Benlliure, V. (2010). Viabilidad del modelo de Encontrar Problemas para evaluar la creatividad en Educación Primaria. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 33, 335–349.
  • Romo, M., Alfonso-Benlliure, V. y Sánchez Ruiz, M. J. (2008). Test de Creatividad Infantil. Madrid: Tea Ediciones.
  • Rosenblatt, E. y Winner, E. (1988). The art of children’s drawings. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 22, 3–15.
  • Runco, M. (1994). Problem finding, problem solving and creativity. Norwood. NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
  • Runco, M. (2003). Idea evaluation, divergent thinking, and creativity. En M. A. Runco (Ed.), Critical creative processes (pp. 69–74). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
  • Runco, M. A. (2008). Commentary: Divergent thinking is not synonymous with creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 93–96.
  • Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Emergence in creativity and development. En R. K. Sawyer,V. John-Steiner, S. Moran, R. Sternberg, D. H. Feldman, M. Csikszentmihalyi, y J. Nakamura (Eds.), Creativity and Development (pp. 12–60). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Sawyer, R. K. (2012). Explaining creativity: The science of human innovation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Smith, G. J. y Carlsson, I. M. (1983). Creativity in early and middle school years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 167–195.
  • Smith, G. J. y Carlsson, I. M. (1985). Creativity in middle and late school years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 329–343.
  • Smith, G. J. y Carlsson, I. M. (1990). The creative process: A functional model based on empirical studies from early childhood to middle age. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
  • Smolucha, F. (1992). A reconstruction of Vygotsky’s theory of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 5.
  • Sternberg, R. (1988). The nature of creativity. Contemporary psychological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sternberg, R. (1999). Handbook of creativity. New York. NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sternberg, R. y Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51, 677–688.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1968). A longitudinal examination of the fourth-grade slump in creativity. Gifted Child Quarterly, 12, 195–199.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–252.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1974). Norms-technical manual: Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Lexington, MA: Personel Press/Ginn.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing. En R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity, Contemporary psychological perspectives (pp. 43–75). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1990). The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms - technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1998). The Torrance tests of creative thinking norms - technical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
  • Treffinger, D. J. (1985). Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. En J. V. Mitchell Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (pp. 1632–1634). Lincoln: University of Nebraska. Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
  • Urban, K. K. (1991). On the development of creativity in children. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 177–191.
  • Urban, K. K. (2004). Assessing Creativity: The Test for Creative Thinking-Drawing Production (TCT-DP). The Concept, Application, Evaluation and International Studies. Psychology Science, 46, 387–397.
  • Urban, K. K. y Jellen, H. G. (1996). Test for Creative Thinking - Drawing Production (TCTDP). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1930-2004). Imagination and creativity in childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42(1), 4–84.
  • Wallach, M. A. y Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children. New York, NY: Holt.
  • Wakefield, J. F. (1991). The outlook for creativity tests. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 25, 184–193.
  • Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. Hoboken, New York, NY: John Wiley.
  • Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper and Brothers. (Trad. Cast. de L. Wolfson, El pensamiento productivo. Barcelona: Paidós, 1991).
  • Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W. y Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 24–37.